First described in the 16th Century
by Italian anatomist Costanzo Varoli, the pons consists of
two parts: the tegmentum, which contains the reticular formation,
and the pontine
nuclei. It connects the medulla with the cerebellum, while
its thick ascending and descending fibers, "gangly giants,"1 reach
from brain stem to neocortex. Its name is derived from the
Latin for "bridge."
The prehistory of
bridge-building may begin with humans swinging across chasms on vines.
In the 16th Century, Spanish invaders of South America reported seeing
Peruvian suspension bridges built from fibers "woven
into cables of the thickness of a man's body." How old the
technology was by that time is not known. Besides being triumphs
of human ingenuity, bridges are places of imaginative
space. Were ancient bridges the result of pontine fibers dreamed
by ancient shaman/engineers?
The pons secretes a class of neurotransmitters
called the amines, the best described of which are norepinephaline
and serotonin, which serve to allow the higher brain to pontificate
over the organism. During REM (dream) sleep, these substances are
drastically reduced, replaced by the cholinergic neurotransmitters--secreted
by another part of the pons--, especially acetycholine, a chemical
commonly found the junctions of muscles and nerves. While we
dream, acetycholine inhibits kinesthesis, except for the eyes. (REM
is an acronym for Rapid Eye Movement.) The function of such eye-movement
seems announce the passive viewing of images.
Michel
Jouvet, who pioneered research into the neurobiology of dreams,
calls REM sleep "paradoxical," because, although the
cerebral cortex is flooded with images, it cannot link them to
the body, which is atonic. Jouvet calls dreaming a third state, "a
new form of an age-old concept--that
of the Upanishads of Hindu mythology--according
to which the human brain alternates between waking, sleeping
without dreams, and sleeping with dreams." 2
Via the amygdala, pontine cells can generate anxiety,
initiating the common nightmare of being pursued but not being able
to run, by sending strong signals to the cerebral cortex, where sensate
impressions and memories would normally meet, but are now hampered
by the dearth of amines. With this in mind, a leading investigator
in the field wrote that "dreams are the product of our cortex's
efforts to do the best it can under very difficult circumstances."3
Ironically, while
working on this trace of the journey, I wasn't able to remember
my dreams, even if I tried at the moment of waking up. Something
usual for me. I knew that I had dreamed, but of what I couldn't
recall. Then impressions formed while awake began to substitute
for dreams. Thus I arrived at a place dreamed as if I were still
dreaming. Chuang-tzu's butterfly, or Psyche's
honey?
(1)
J. Hooper and D. Teresi, The three-Pound Universe. New York,
1986. p.293.
(2) M. Jouvet, The Paradox of Sleep. Cambridge, MA., 1999. p.37.
(3) J.A. Hobson, "Order From Chaos." In, R. Conlan, Editor. States
of Mind. New York, 1999. p.191.